Who introduced the level of moral reasoning explanation?
Kohlberg, was the first researcher to apply the concept of moral reasoning to offending behaviour.
He proposed that people’s decisions and judgements on issues of right and wrong can be summarised in a stage theory of moral reasoning.
The higher the stage= the more sophisticated reasoning.
Level 1- Preconventional morality
Stage 1: Punishment orientation
Rules are obeyed to avoid punishment
Stage 2: Instrumental orientation of personal gain
Rules are obeyed for personal gain
Level 2: Conventional morality
Stage 3: ’Good boy’ or ‘good girl’ orientation
Rules are obeyed for approval
Stage 4: Maintenance of the social order
Rules are obeyed to maintain the social order
Level 3: Postconventional morality
Stage 5: Morality of contract and individual rights
Rules are obeyed if they are important; democratic rules are challenged if they infringe on the rights of others.
Stage 6: Morality of conscience
The individuals establishes his or own rules in accordance with a personal set of ethical principles
What did Kohlberg based is theory of moral reasoning on?
Kohlberg based his theory on people’s responses to a series of moral dilemmas.
Such as Heinz Dilemma
What does the Heinz Dilemma study suggest?
Kohlberg et al, found using his moral dilemmas, a group of violent youths were at a significantly lower level of moral development than non-violent youths, even after controlling for social background.
This suggests that offenders tend to show a lower level of moreal reasoning than non-offenders
What level would criminals likely to be at in Kohlberg’s model?
Offenders are more likely to be classified at the pre-conventional level of Kohlbergs model
The pre-conventional level is characterised by a need to avoid punishment and gain rewards, less mature and child-like reasoning.
What behaviours are associated with a higher level in Kohlberg’smoral reasoning stages?
Sympathise more with the rights of others
Honesty
Generosity
Non-violence
Research support for Kohlberg’slevel of moral reasoning stages explanation
Palmer and Hollin, compared moral reasoning in 332non-offenders and 126convicted offenders, using the Socio Moral Reflection Measure Short Form (SRM-SF) which contains 11 moral dilemma-related questions e.g keeping a promise for a friend
The results found offenders showed less mature moral reasoning than non-offenders.
This suggests that Kohlberg’s predictions are accurate as lower moral reasoning is associated with offending behaviour.
Moral thinking VS Moral behaviour are not the SAME
Critics of Kohlberg’s research have suggested that the moral dilemma techniques may be a poor predictor of real life behaviour.
For instance, the Heinz dilemma is hypothetical and may not reflect the moral decisions that someone would exercise in real life.
This means the technique is low in external validity and it may be more useful to understand moral behaviour as not everyone who had criminal thoughts will act on them.
Moral reasoning depends on the type of offence
Thornton and Reid found that people who commit crimes for financial gain e.g robbery were more likely to show pre-conventional moral reasoning, than those convicted of impulsive crimes e.g assault.
Pre-conventional moral reasoning tends to be associated with crimes in which offenders believe they have a good chance of evading punishment.
This suggests that Kohlberg’s theory may not apply to all forms of crime.
Cognitive distortions
Refer to faulty, biased and irrational ways of thinking that mean were perceive ourselves, other people and the world inaccurately and usually negatively.
2 examples of cognitive distortions
Hostile attribution bias
Minimalisation
Hostile attribution bias
The tendency to judge ambiguous situations or the actions of others as aggressive and/or threatening when in reality they may not be.
How does hostile attribution bias lead to violence?
Offenders may mis-read non-aggressive cues, such as being looked at and this may trigger disproportionate, often violent responses.
Research support for hostile attribution bias
Schonenberg and Jusyte,
Presented 55violent offenders with images of emotionally ambiguous facial expressions.
When compared to a non-aggressive matched control group, the violent offenders were more likely to perceive the images as angry and hostile.
Research support for the roots of hostile attribution bias
Dodge and Frame, showed children a video clip of ‘ambiguous provocation’ where the intension was neither hostile or accidental.
Children who had been identified as ‘aggressive’ prior to the study interpreted the situation as more hostile than those classed as ‘non-aggressive’
Minimalisation
A type of deception that involves downplaying the significance of an event or an emotion. A common strategy when dealing with feelings of guilt
Example of minimalisation
A burglar may describe themselves as ‘doing a job’ or ‘supporting my family’ as a way of minimising the seriousness of their offences.
Research support for minimalisation
Barbaree, found among 26 incarcerated rapists, 54% denied they had committed an offence at all and a further 40% minimise the harm they had caused to the victim.
Real world application for cognitive distortions
Application to therapy
CBT aims to challenge irrational thinking. In the case of offending behaviour, offenders are encouraged to ‘face up’ to what they have done and establish a less distorted view of their actions
Harkins et al, suggests that reduced incidence of denial and minimalisation in therapy is highly associated with the reduced risk of reoffending
This suggests that the theory of cognitive distortions has practical value.
Cognitive distortion theories do not explain or help predict future offender behaviour
There is a large body of evidence in psychology which suggests that attitudes or beliefs are weakly correlated with actual behaviour.
We cannot assume that just because someone has distorted thinking that they will become an offender.
Therefore if we want to predict whether someone will go on to offend we need to understand, rather than describe the causes of their behaviour
How does cognitive distortions depend on the type of offence
Howitt and Sheldon gathered questionnaires responses from sexual offenders. Contrary to what researchers predicted, they found that non-contact sex offended (accessed images on the internet) used more cognitive distortions than contact sex offenders (had previously abused children)
Those who had previous history of offending were also more likely to use distortions as a justification.
This suggests that distortions are not used in the same way by all offenders.