is any behaviour of benefit to someone else - it includes actions that are cooperative, affectionate or helpful to others. such behaviour may or may not be costly to the person engaging in such behaviour - it is often as beneficial to that person as to the person being assisted
altruism;
is an especially important type of prosocial behaviour. it is based on a desire to help someone else rather than on possible rewards for the person doing the rewarding.
it is assumed that altruism depends on empathy - the ability to share another's emotions and understand their point of view
do young children exhibit prosocial behaviour?
Freud and Piaget; children's tendency to engage in antisocial rather than prosocial behaviour.
Schaffer (1996) "the child emerged from these accounts as a selfish, self-centred, aggressive, and uncooperative being, with little ... understanding of anyone else's needs"
Zahn-Waxler et al (1992) found children between 13-20 months showed empathic concern on 10% of occasions when someone was in distress (not caused by the child). this doubled among children of 23-25 months.
Zahn-Waxler also found young children engaged in prosocial behaviour in response to someone's distress. (marked increased with age in response to distress NOT caused by the child)
Svetlova et al (2010): studied 3 kinds of prosocial behaviour in 18-30 months.
instrumental helping: assisting someone to achieve an action based goal e.g. finding a toy
empathic helping: showing concern about another person
altruistic helping: giving up an objects owned by the child.
Svetlova cont.: children showed more instrumental helping, less empathic helping and little altruistic helping - altruistic helping was rarely costly and mostly produced in response to an adult's direct request rather than spontaneous
Carpendale et al (2015): much "helping" behaviour in young children is not based on cooperation and altruism. instead it indicates an interest in participating in the activity of others.
Daniel et al (2016): children between 18-54 months found that their mother's warmth and the father's warmth were both associated with increased prosocial behaviour by their offspring.
Knafo et al (2011): 45% of individual differences in prosocial behaviour in young twins were due to genetic factors.
in young children, 24-36 months, 25% of individual differences in empathy depended on genetic factors. the amount of prosocial behaviour shown by children depended on part of their empathy level.
stages in the development of empathy by Hoffman.
evolutionary perspective; high motivation to keep their genes alive (a conscious and unconscious want)
inclusivefitness - organisms' need to reproduce to keep their genes going
kinselection - organisms are selected to favour their own offspring and other genetically related people.
evolutionary perspective cont.:
Tomasello et al (2012): interdependence hypothesis - altruistic behaviour developed when our ancestors discovered the benefits of mutualistic collaboration (cooperation between people mutually beneficial).
research shows the importance of genetic relatedness or kinship, especially in life-and-death situations
Fellner and Marshall (1981) found 86% of people were willing to be a kidney donor for their children, 67% for their parents, and 50% for a sibling
Hackman et al (2015) emotional closeness and genetic relatedness.
as predicted; the amount sacrificed was greater for biological kin than friends when the effects of emotional closeness were removed
Fehr and Fischbacher (2003) the role played by individuals' desire to have a reputation for altruism. participants decided whether to help another person who could not reciprocate that help - help was provided by 74% of those who would gain a reputation for altruism.
Roos et al (2014) third-party punishment is most effective in groups characterised by strong social ties (individuals who interact frequently) and low mobility (individuals cannot easily switch groups)
Roos et al (2014) cont.: recipients of third-party punishment have the strongest motive to cooperate with other members of their group and to behave altruistically. he discussed support for their argument with different groups.
empathy-altruism hypothesis and negative-state relief model;
people also behave altruistically towards non-relatives = reciprocal altruism "ill scratch your back if you scratch mine"
Trivers (1971) argued reciprocal altruism is most likely to be found in two conditions;
costs of helping are low and benefits are high
can identify those who cheat by receiving help but not helping in return
empathy-altruism cont.: many individuals behave altruistically even when those they help are unlikely to reciprocate.
Fehr and Fischbacher (2003): it allows those individuals to gain a reputation for behaving altruistically - this increases the changes they will be assisted by others in the future.
empathy-altruism cont.:
Empathy-altruism hypothesis (Batson):
Negative state relief model (Cialdini)
societal norms: involve treating others fairly e.g. distribution norm (goods should be distributed equally).
Third-party punishment e.g. (this reduces selfishness and increase cooperation)
competition and personal success found in individualistic cultures reduces cooperation and altruism e.g. USA.
collectivistic cultures e.g. Kenya, on the group's needs rather than the individual's means children are often given major family responsibilities that develop altruistic behaviour
Fijneman et al (1996): living in collectivistic cultures expect more help from others than those living in individualistic cultures. both kinds of cultures are similar in that individuals anticipate giving only a little more help than they expect to receive.
how to encourage children to be more prosocial;
television; Valkenburg et al (2016) limitations; children who are already prosocial are more likely to watch prosocial tv. the affects of prosocial tv are short-term (rather weak or even non-existent), Sagotsky et al, 1981
how to encourage children to be more prosocial cont.:
video games; Greitemeyer and Mugge (2014) playing prosocial video games increased prosocial emotions, cognitions and behaviour and also reduced aggressive behaviour. effects were reasonably consistent although relatively small in size.
studies show an association between playing prosocial games and prosocial behaviour.
Valkenburg et al (2016); playing prosocial games enhances prosocial behaviour to that exhibiting prosocial behaviour leads to increased game playing
parental influence; parents are easily the most important adults in children's lives.
Schaffer (1996): several types of parental behaviour are of special value in teaching children to be prosocial and altruistic
parental influence cont.:
provisions of clear and explicit guidelines: e.g. you must not hit others it will hurt them
emotional convictions: guidelines to children should be given in a fairly emotional way
parental modeling: parents should behave altruistically towards their children
empathic and warm parenting: parents should have a good understanding of their children's needs and emotions
parental influence - provisions of clear and explicit guidelines
Krevans and Gibbs (1996): children exhibited more prosocial behaviour when their mothers repeatedly told them to consider the effects of their behaviour on others
parental influence - emotional convictions
children showed altruistic behaviour twice as often when their mother frequently used emotional explanations
parental influence - parental modeling
Burleseon and Kunkel (2002): mothers' comforting skills predicted their children's emotional support skills
parental influence - empathic and warm parenting
Robinson et al (1994): children having a warm and loving relationship with their parents were most likely to show prosocial behaviour
genetic importance;
Knafo-Noam et al (2015): did a twin study and discovered 69% of individual difference in children's prosociality were due to genetic factors.