Social groups

Cards (18)

  • Petyt (1985)
    • Petyt (1985) looked at aitch-dropping and the RP /ʌ / (‘u’) sound in words like good and the / ʊ / (‘uh’) sound in words like ‘put’ in the city of Bradford. 
    • He found that:
    • The lower the social class, the more likely the speaker was to drop the aitch.
    • The socially aspirational speakers incorrectly used the sounds in words like cushion pronouncing it with the / ʊ / sound instead of the / ʌ / sound.
  • Peter Trudgill (1974) - research aim
    • Peter Trudgill's (1974Norwich study looked at the pronunciation of the words ‘walking’ and ‘talking’. Trudgill was looking for the non-standard forms walkin’ and talkin’ (this is called ‘g-dropping’ or a ‘g-drop’). 
    • He also looked at the use of the ‘-s’ in verbs, like ‘he goes to school’ and the non-standard ‘he go to school’.
    • The research’s aim was to see if there was a correlation between class, gender and the use of the non-standard forms.
  • Peter Trudgill (1974) - Findings
    • He asked participates to state how often they used standard and non-standard forms. He found that:
    • Women of all classes over-reported the number of standard forms they used which showed that they cared about the overt prestige. 
    • Men over-reported their use of non-standard forms, showing the covert prestige they desire. 
    • Women used more standard forms than men in a direct class-for-class comparison.
    • The higher the class, the more use of standard forms.
    • The research concludes that class is more of a determiner than gender.
  • Basil Bernstein (1970s)
    • Conducted research into the way classes talk. 
    • He asked groups of boys to describe a comic strip containing a football smashing a window. 
    • From this, he defines two codes – restricted and elaborated.
    • Restricted code is inherently deictic. It has a relaxed syntax (word order) and uses many simple coordinating conjunctions.
    • Elaborated code is practically the opposite. It uses a standard syntax, more subordinate clauses and subordinating conjunctions.
  • Basil Bernstein (1970s) - Findings
    • Bernstein claims that whilst middle-class children can use both codes, it is rare to see a lower-class child using anything other than restricted code. 
    • Saying this, both classes did understand both codes when spoken to them. 
    • There are obviously times when it is advantageous to be able to use both codes. 
    • For example, social situations and bonding situations are helped through restricted code, but formal events are helped by elaborated code.
  • William Labov (1969)
    • His research featured black boys speaking in what would be classed as restricted code, but with a logical argument. 
    • This suggests that there is an issue with linguistically articulating ideas rather than there being a cognitive deficit.
  • William Labov -research aim
    • investigated the use of the post-vocalic rhotic-r (the ‘r’ sound that appears after vowels) in words like ‘banter’ and ‘mark’.
    • To investigate this, he visited three stores in New York of different price and fashion status: Saks, Macy’s and Klein’s
    • Labov asked a question about the location of a department which would evoke the answer ‘fourth floor’. He would then tell the assistant that he had not heard them and ask them to repeat. 
    • At this point, the speech stops becoming spontaneous and starts becoming more conscious.
  • William Labov - Findings
    His research details that:
    • The lower middle class were most aware of the overt prestige (biggest change from casual to spontaneous speech). This is indicative of their need to social climb.
    • The lower classes tested were more aware of the prestige form and so more likely to change the way they spoke when consciously speaking.
    • The upper middle class changed the least, indicative of their already higher social status.
  • Jenny Cheshire
    looked at 11 non-standard forms used by children against their adherence to the law on the playground at a school in Reading.
    These features included:
    • Multiple negation
    • Non-standard use of ‘what’ (e.g. ‘is this the book what we are learning about?') and 'ain’t’ being used as a copular verb (‘you ain’t the teacher’).
    • All children that approved of criminal activities within their peer group were more likely to use non-standard forms.
    • More boys than girls approved. This means we can deduce that non-standard variations are conscious choices in language.
  • Jenny Cheshire (con)
    • There is a clear prestige around standard and non-standard forms. 
    • Those who abide by the law clearly gain their prestige in an overt way from behaving. 
    • The covert prestige of using the non-standard forms fits in with the breaking of laws – these children rebel against the rules in the same way they rebel against the social norms.
  • Penelope Eckert
    Penelope Eckert researched vowel sounds in Detroit (the /e/ and the /ʌ/ sounds, so that the word ‘flesh’ would sound like ‘flush’) and also negative concord (this is the same as multiple negation).
    • She defines two key groups – the Jocks and the Burnouts. 
    • The Jocks are school-centred and engaged in school-sphered activities. 
    • The Burnouts were the opposite of this – concentrating on gaining a job in the local workforce. They engaged in activities in the neighbourhood area.
  • Penelope Eckert (findings)
    • The Jocks used a significantly higher proportion of the standard form. Their vowels were conservative and their grammatical constructions were practically all standard (no negative concord).
    • The Burnouts were focussed on the local surroundings and so used the Detroit vowels more severely. They also used significantly more negative concord.
  • Eckert - Age
    there are three main types of age:
    • Chronological – the physical time someone has been alive.
    • Biological – the maturity of the body.
    • Social – the way someone is developed in society.
  • Emma Moore
    Emma Moore’s 2010 research into social groups at a school in Bolton looked at the non-standard use of ‘were’ in sentences like ‘it was in maths, weren’t it?’. Her research looks at four groups:
    Eden Valley girls - higher class, focused on school, lived further away from other girls, used the standard form (‘was’) almost all of the time.
    Populars - not focused on school, engaged in rebellious activities. The group did not always use the non-standard form. But three girls were members of a higher social class – these girls did not use the non-standard form at all.
  • Emma Moore (part 2)
    Townies - breakaway group from populars, also rebellious. These girls often made use of the non-standard form. They were a mix of upper-working-class and lower-middle-class girls in the group. But they did have working-class boys in their social group.
    Geeks - focused on school and disliked non-standard form. However, 4 that did use it were all lower class
  • Sali Tagliamonte
    • Teenagers have gained a mastery in a new set of registers.
    • ‘Like’ as a quotative verb is on the increase in anecdotes.
    • Almost a quarter of all adjectives are intensified.
  • Anna-Brita Stenström (2014)
    there are many features that make-up teen speak. A few of these include:
    • Slang.
    • Conversational overlapping.
    • Shortening of words.
    • Taboo and expletives.
  • De Klerk
    teenagers have a freedom which allows them to rebel against the linguistic norms to mark themselves as different to others.