Pre-sleep monologue made by young children while in bed
Crib talk
Starts around one-and-a-half years and usually ends by about two-and-a-half years, though children can continue longer
Consists of conversational discourse with turn-taking, often containing semantically and syntactically coherent question-answer sequences
May contain wordplay and bits of song and nursery rhyme
Varieties of crib talk
Enactment sequences
Proto-narrative
Problem focused
Enactment sequences
Occur most commonly in early monologues, done in a lowtone, concerning language to bring about action, occurring when playing with toys and describingongoingplay
Proto-narrative
A child creates a story about events that have happened or imagined in temporal causal sequences, can be as short as five words or as long as 150, may include reciting of stories that have been read to them
Problem focused
Concern what happened in the past, what will happen in the future and how events are organized, incorporate descriptions used by others to enable prediction
Functions of crib talk
Provides a practice space for developing complex connected discourse
Aids a child to use language as a tool to categorize, explain and know the world
Clarifies what may originally have been problematic or troublesome
Crib talk is more complex than that done by children in interactions with others, due to the freedom to control what they say and not have their cognitive abilities stretched by having to work out how to respond to what someone has just said</b>
Crib talk lacks the self-regulatory instructions of private speech, which usually starts after 3 and ends about 7
The children studied in crib talk research are atypical in that they are the offspring of researchers or their close colleagues, and so are from highly educated backgrounds
The child studied by Katherine Nelson was highly precocious in her language abilities, which raises questions about the generality of findings on that one child
Other studies on crib talk
Ruth Hirsch Weir on her son Anthony (1962)
Stan Kuczaj on 14 children between 15-30 months (1983)
Katherine Nelson on Emily (1989)
Private speech
Self-talk children (and adults) may use to guide actions and aid in thinking, considered to be self-directed regulation and communication with the self
Private speech
Can be categorised as overt or covert, and related or unrelated to the task the child is engaged in
Children progress from overt speech to covert speech as they mature
Has a developmental role contingent on observation of adult speech and practice of linguistic skills through guided participation with adults
Vygotsky's view on private speech
Thought is internalized language, private speech is the critical transitional process between speaking with others and thinking for oneself
Vygotsky's stages of speech development: 1) Social speech, 2) Egocentric speech, 3) Inner speech
Egocentric speech
The type of speech found in 3-7 year olds, where children talk to themselves to guide their own behaviour
Inner speech
The final stage of speech development, internal, soundless speech used by older children and adults to direct thinking and behaviour
Halliday's functions of language
Heuristic function - language used to explore, learn and discover
Imaginative function - language used to invent dialogues, worlds of fantasy, stories and so on
Piaget's view on egocentric speech
Since the child is the centre of the universe, the assumption is that people will understand what is said no matter how it is said, so there is no effort made to tailor the speech for a particular listener
Types of egocentric speech
Echolalia - repeating sounds, words, phrases and/or sentences for the fun of saying it
Monologues - running discourse, usually occurring when a child is playing
Social (or Dual) Monologues - two or more children vocalizing in close proximity but not interacting
Piaget notes that the incidence of egocentric speech slowly dies out between 2 and 7 years as that of social speech increases
Covert private speech
Internal dialogue, not spoken out loud
Overt private speech
Spoken out loud, can be related or unrelated to the task the child is engaged in
Functions of private speech
Can help children regulate their behavior, solve problems, and learn new skills; serves as a bridge between inner thought and outer communication
Progression of private speech
Children typically progress from overt to covert private speech as they mature
Developmental role of private speech
Contingent on observation of adult speech and practice of linguistic skills through guided participation with adults