social influence

Cards (76)

  • Social influence looks at how people’s beliefs and behaviours are affected by people around them. A Level psychology looks at the following exaemples of social influence:
    • Conformity: Doing what the group does
    • Obedience: Doing what an authority figure tells you to do
  • Conformity, also known as majority influence, is when a person changes their beliefs and behaviour to fit – or conform – to those of a group. Kelman (1958) identifies the following 3 types of conformity, going from weakest to strongest:
  • compliance
    Compliance is the weakest type of conformity. It is where a person publicly changes their behaviour and beliefs to fit that of a group and avoid disapproval. However, privately, the person does not accept the behaviours and beliefs of the group – they just comply with them.An example of compliance would be pretending to like a film you dislike so as not to stand out from a group who all really love that film.
  • Identification
    Identification is a stronger type of conformity than compliance because it involves the person both publicly and privately changing their behaviour and beliefs to fit that of a group they want to be part of. However, the person only identifies with these beliefs as long as they are associated with the group – upon leaving the group, the original behaviours and beliefs return.An example of identification would be adopting the same music and fashion tastes as your friendship group. When you move away, though, you revert back to your old clothes and music.
  • Internalisation
    Internalisation is the strongest type of conformity. It is where a person both publicly and privately changes their behaviour and beliefs to those of a group – but permanently. So, unlike identification, individuals who internalise beliefs and behaviours maintain those beliefs and behaviours even after leaving the social group.An example of internalisation would be a person who undergoes a genuine religious conversion. This person will still pray and believe in God even if they move away from the social group of their church.
  • SOLOMON ASCH: CONFORMITY EXPERIMENTS
  • Look at the line in the box on the left. Which of the lines in the box on the right – A, B, or C – is closest in length to it? This was the question put to participants in Asch (1955). The aim of these experiments was to find out the extent to which people would conform to an obviously wrong majority consensus.
    • The procedure for the experiments was as follows:
    • 123 male participants were told they were taking part in a study of visual perception.
    • Participants were put in groups with between 7 and 9 confederates (i.e. fake subjects pretending to be part of the experiment too).
    • Each participant completed 18 trials where they would be shown the sets of lines above (A, B, or C) and then asked which one was closest to the original line.
    • In the 12 critical trials, the confederates would all give the same wrong answer – the participant was always asked to give their answer last (or second to last) so as to hear the group’s answers first.
    • The control group for this experiment consisted of 36 participants. In the control trials, participants were asked the same question as above – but this time alone.
  • The results of the experiments were as follows:
    • Across all critical trials, participants conformed to the incorrect group consensus 32% of the time.
    • 75% of participants conformed to at least one incorrect answer
    • 5% of participants conformed to every incorrect answer
    • This is compared to an error rate of just 0.04% in the control trials.
  • After the experiment, Asch conducted interviews with the participants. Conforming subjects gave 3 explanations of their conformity:
    • Distortion of perception: A small few subjects actually came to perceive the majority estimates as correct and were completely unaware of their mistake.
    • Distortion of judgement: The majority of conforming subjects were aware of their mistake but did not trust their own judgement and instead decided that the majority was correct.
    • Distortion of action: These subjects were aware of – and trusted – their judgement that the majority was wrong but nevertheless gave the wrong answer so as not to stand out and be different.
  • VARIABLES AFFECTING CONFORMITY
  • UNANIMITY
    Participants’ conformity declined from 32% to 5.5% when one ‘partner’ confederate was instructed to give the correct answer and go against the incorrect answer of the majority.
    Asch’s findings are consistent with other research which finds conformity rates decline when the majority answer is not unanimous. In other words, if the majority all agree, the participant is more likely to conform to the group than if there is some disagreement.
  • GROUP SIZE
    Increasing the size of the group tended to increase conformity – up to a point. In trials with just one confederate and one participant, conformity rates were low. Increasing the number of confederates to 2 also increased conformity to 12.8% and increasing the number of confederates to 3 increased conformity even further to 32% (the same as the original study). However, adding extra confederates (4, 8, or 16) beyond this did not increase conformity.
  • Asch’s findings on conformity and majority size have been replicated in other studies, but other studies suggest conformity continues to increase with majority size beyond this.
  • DIFFICULTY
    Increasing the difficulty of the task was also found to increase conformity. Asch adjusted the lengths of the lines in the study above to make it either more easy or more difficult to see which line was closest in length to the original line. If the difference between the incorrect answer and the correct answer was very small (and thus harder to notice), participants were more likely to conform to the incorrect answers of the majority.
  • OTHER VARIABLES AFFECTING CONFORMITY
    In addition to Asch’s experiments, psychological research suggests other variables that affect conformity include:
    • Mood: Various studies have found correlations between mood and conformity. For example, Tong et al (2008) found that subjects are more likely to conform when they are in a good mood. Further, Dolinski (2001) found that subjecting participants to an “emotional seesaw” makes them more likely to conform.
    • Gender: Some research (e.g. Jenness (1932) and Maslach et al (1987)) suggests women are more likely to conform than men.
    • Culture: A meta-analysis by Smith and Bond (1996) found that conformity is higher among participants in “collectivist” cultures than “individualist” cultures.
  • EXPLANATIONS OF CONFORMITY
    Deutsch and Gerard (1955) explain why people conform by identifying 2 motivating forces: Informational social influence and normative social influence.
  • INFORMATIONAL SOCIAL INFLUENCE (ISI)
    People like to feel that their opinions and beliefs are correct – this is informational social influence. This desire to be correct motivates individuals to act on information provided by members of the group because they believe that information to be true or the correct way to do things. An example of informational social influence is conforming to others’ behaviour at a formal restaurant. You don’t know which cutlery is the correct set to use, so you just copy someone else who seems to know what they’re doing.
  • NORMATIVE SOCIAL INFLUENCE (NSI)
    People want to be accepted by others and not be rejected – this is normative social influence. This desire to fit in motivates individuals to conform to the beliefs and opinions of a group so as not to stand out. The motivation of normative social influence is not a desire to be correct (like ISI), but is instead a desire to be liked and accepted.
    An example of being motivated by normative social influence would be being peer pressured into agreeing with the group’s opinions on politics.
  • CONFORMITY TO SOCIAL ROLES
    Different social situations have different expectations for behaviour – different social norms. These norms give rise to social roles that people play according to the social situation. We play these different roles – i.e. we conform to social roles – so as to behave correctly and appropriately in society. For example, in the role of employee, you are expected to show up on time and do your work. In the role of customer in a shop, you are expected to pay for the items you buy.
  • PHILIP ZIMBARDO: STANFORD PRISON STUDY AIM
    The aim of the Stanford prison study (Zimbardo et al (1973)) was to find out how much people conform to the social roles of prisoner and guard in a prison situation.
  • PROCEDURE
    • Zimbardo and his team converted the basement of the psychology department at Stanford University into a fake prison.
    • 21 male students were selected from a total of 75 participants for their mental stability and lack of antisocial tendencies.
    • These 21 participants were randomly divided into two groups: 10 ‘guards’ and 11 ‘prisoners’
    • Prisoners were arrested by real police and then subjected to real police booking procedures (e.g. fingerprinting and mug shots). They were put in cells in groups of 3 and were confined throughout the experiment.
    • Guards worked in 8 hour shifts and were instructed to refer to the prisoners by their assigned numbers rather than their names. A realistic prison routine was established with meal times, etc.
    • The prisoners wore jackets with their number on, and a chain around one ankle. Guards wore khaki uniforms, mirrored sunglasses to prevent eye contact, and carried handcuffs and wooden batons.
    • The study was scheduled to run for 2 weeks.
  • The results of this observational study were extreme:
    • The guards became increasingly sadistic. For example, they forced the prisoners to continually repeat their assigned numbers and made them go to the toilet in buckets in their cells. As punishment, the guards refused to allow prisoners to empty these buckets, took away their mattresses and made them sleep on the concrete floor, and took away their clothes.
    • The prisoners became increasingly submissive. Many stopped questioning the guards behaviour and sided with the guards against rebellious prisoners
    • After 35 hours, one prisoner began to “act crazy, to scream, to curse, to go into a rage that seemed out of control” and had to be released. Three other prisoners had to be released for similar reasons throughout the duration of the experiment.
    • The guards’ sadism became so harmful that Zimbardo stopped the experiment after just 6 days instead of the scheduled 2 weeks.
  • The results of the prison experiment suggest that people conform to social roles to a significant extent.
    In interviews with participants afterwards, both the prisoners and the guards expressed shock at how out-of-character their behaviour had become. Remember, the participants were selected for their mental stability, yet they behaved in ways that they would ordinarily consider to be wrong (particularly the guards).
  • This supports a situational hypothesis of behaviour over a dispositional one: these people did not necessarily have a sadistic disposition, but were instead conforming to the social roles of the situation.
  • However, it should be noted that not all participants conformed to their roles to the same extent. Some guards did not sadistically exert control and some prisoners did not break down and become totally submissive. This suggests that while situational factors are important, individual dispositions play an important role in behaviour too. Further, some studies designed to replicate Zimbardo’s have challenged his emphasis on social roles.
  • Obedience is when a person complies with – obeys – the orders of an authority figure.
  • STANLEY MILGRAM: OBEDIENCE EXPERIMENTS AIM
    The aim of Milgram’s (1963) obedience study was to investigate the extent to which people obey the orders of an authority figure.
  • The procedure to test for this – variations of which are now termed the Milgram paradigm – was as follows:
    • 40 American male participants aged 20-50 were told they were taking part in an study of the effects of punishment on memory and learning.
    • The confederate ‘experimenter’ (wearing a lab coat to create an impression of authority) told the participant that he had been randomly assigned the role of ‘teacher’ and that another participant (who was another confederate) had been randomly assigned the role of ‘learner’.
    • The experimenter told the participant the test would involve giving increasingly powerful electric shocks to the learner from a machine in the room next door (marked with different voltage levels).
    • The participant watched the learner be strapped into into a chair and have electrodes attached to his body. The participant was also given a 45 volt shock himself so that he believed everything was real.
    • The participant teacher (in the room next door) was instructed to teach the learner a list of word pairs. For each wrong answer from the learner, the teacher had to give him an electric shock. These electric shocks increased in power with each wrong answer – starting at 15 volts and increasing by 15 volts each time all the way up to 450 volts.
    • Once electric shocks reached 150 volts, the learner began to protest. These protests (pre-recorded and played via a tape recorder) increased in intensity with the increasing voltage. At 315 volts, the learner screamed in pain. After 330 volts, the learner went silent.
    • If the participant asked to stop the experiment, the experimenter would reply with one of four successive verbal prods:
    • “Please continue” or “please go on”
    • “The experiment requires that you continue”
    • “It is absolutely essential that you continue”
    • “You have no other choice, you must go on”
  • The results of the experiment were as follows:
    • 26 out of 40 participants (65%) administered shocks all the way up to the maximum of 450 volts.
    • 40 out of 40 participants (100%) administered shocks up to 300 volts.
    • Most participants displayed physical symptoms of discomfort at what they were doing such as sweating, twitching, and nervously laughing. 3 participants suffered seizures from the stress of what they were doing.
  • Milgram’s study was in part motivated by a desire to understand why Nazi soldiers in World War 2 acted how they did. For example, shortly before Milgram’s study, Adolf Eichmann – a senior Nazi officer responsible for deporting Jews to ghettos and concentration camps – defended his actions at trial by repeatedly saying “I was only following orders”.
  • Milgram wanted to know if the German people had a uniquely obedient disposition that explained their behaviour. The study suggests not: American people will also obey the demands of an authority figure even if it means going against their moral compass.