cognitive

Cards (11)

  • moral development
    - Kohlberg proposed that as children get older their decisions and judgements about right and wrong become more sophisticated
    - a person's level of reasoning affects their behaviour
  • Kohlberg's level of moral reasoning
    level 1: pre-conventional
    punishment orientation
    instrumental/personal gain
    level 2: conventional
    good boy/girl orientation
    • maintenance social order
    level 3: post-conventional
    individual rights
    • morality of conscience
  • offenders at lower level
    - Kohlberg et al. (1973) used a moral dilemma technique and found offenders tend to be at the pre-conventional level, whereas non-offenders progress higher
    pre-conventional level is characterised by:
    • a need to avoid punishment and gain rewards
    • less mature, childlike reasoning
    offenders may commit crime if they can get away with it or gain rewards
  • offenders more egocentric and show less empathy
    - research shows that offenders are often self-centred and display poorer social perspective-taking skills (Chandler 1973)
    - individuals who reason at a higher level tend to empathise more and exhibit behaviours such as honesty, generosity and non-violence
  • strength of moral reasoning
    P - evidence supporting the role of moral reasoning
    E - Palmer and Hollin (1998) compared moral reasoning of offenders and non-offenders on a SRM-SF scale
    E - offenders showed less mature moral reasoning than the non-offenders group
    L - this is consistent with Kohlberg's theory and suggests his theory of criminality has validity
  • limitation of moral reasoning
    P - may depend on type of offence
    E - Thornton and Reid (1982) found that people whose crimes were for financial gain were more likely to show pre-conventional level than impulsive crime
    E - pre-conventional moral reasoning tends to be associated with crimes in which offenders believe they have a good chance of evading punishment
    L - suggests that Kohlberg's theory may not apply to all forms of crime
  • cognitive distortions
    - cognitive distortions are errors or biases in information processing characterised by faulty thinking
    - we all occasionally exhibit faulty thinking, but research shows this is a much more typical way for offenders to interpret their behaviour and justify their actions
  • hostile attribution bias
    - Schonenberg and Jusyte (2014) found violent offenders were more likely than non-offenders to perceive ambiguous facial expressions as angry and hostile
    - offenders misread non-aggressive cues and this can trigger a disproportionate and violent response
  • minimalisation
    - minimalisation reduces a person's sense of guilt
    - this is particularly likely in sex offenders: Barbaree (1991) found 54% rapists denied they had committed an offence at all and a further 40% minimised the harm they had caused to the victim
  • strength of cognitive distortions
    P - application to therapy
    E - in cognitive behaviour therapy, offenders are helped to 'face up' to what they have done and have a less distorted view of actions
    E - studies (Harkins et al. 2010) suggest that reduced denial and minimalisation in therapy is associated with less reoffending
    L - suggests that the theory of cognitive distortions has practical value
  • limitation of cognitive distortions
    P - depends on type of offence
    E - Howitt and Sheldon (2007) found that non-contact sex offenders used more cognitive distortions than contact sex offenders
    E - those who had a previous history of offending were also more likely to use distortions as a justification for their behaviour
    L - suggests that cognitive distortions are not used in the same way by all offenders