Babies like to be near their carers and do not like to be separated, as the bond provides a sense of security and comfort influencing psychological, neurological, physical, and cognitive development
Interactional synchrony in caregiver-infant interactions is when two people move in the same pattern, coordinating speech and movement, sharing a sense of timing and imitating each other's behaviors and emotions
Practical applications of research on caregiver-infant interactions include hospitals placing mothers and babies in the same room after birth, leading to stronger bond formation
Attachment can be defined as an emotional relationship between two people seeking closeness and feeling more secure in the presence of the attachment figure
Two features of caregiver-infant interaction outlined from Abdul's conversation: interactional synchrony - adults and babies respond in time to sustain communication, and reciprocity/turn-taking - interaction flows both ways between adult and infant
Schaffer and Emerson found that 3% of children formed their primary attachment with their fathers, while 27% of fathers were the joint first object of attachment
Lorenz's study showed that animals will imprint to any object present during the critical period, suggesting imprinting is not limited to a certain object
One critique of Lorenz's study is that research has failed to support the concept of a 'critical period', as some studies found that animals could still imprint after being isolated for a period of time
Harlow's research on attachment in monkeys raised ethical issues due to distress caused, such as diarrhea and long-lasting emotional harm, questioning the ethicality of the study
Animal studies, like Harlow's and Lorenz's, attempt to generalize their conclusions to humans, but human behavior is more determined by conscious thought than animals, making it challenging to extrapolate animal research to humans
Operant conditioning in attachment involves processes like positive reinforcement (e.g., when a baby cries and gets food) and negative reinforcement (e.g., when a child is fed, the unpleasant sensation of hunger stops)
Primary reinforcers in attachment are biological, like food, water, or pleasure, while secondary reinforcers are learned through association with primary reinforcers, like money or verbal praise
Classical conditioning in attachment, as seen in Pavlov's dogs, involves the stages where an infant learns to associate pleasure solely with the caregiver, without the need for food
Schaffer and Emerson's research challenged the claim that attachments are based on feeding, showing that sensitive responsiveness may be more important
Fox's research suggested that attachment was not solely based on the reward of receiving food, questioning the validity of learning theory in explaining attachment
Bowlby's theory of attachment emphasizes sensitivity and responsiveness to a child's needs, contrasting with learning theory's focus on the role of food in reinforcing attachment behavior
Schaffer and Emerson's findings that multiple attachments were the norm and that sensitive responsiveness may be more important than food support Bowlby's theory of attachment
Insecure-avoidant children seem unconcerned when the mother leaves, while insecure-resistant children show intense distress
During reunion, insecure-avoidant children show little reaction, while insecure-resistant children may cling to their mother but show ambivalent behavior
The behavior of a child with insecure-avoidant attachment type would show little concern when the mother leaves and little reaction upon her return
In contrast, a child with insecure-resistant attachment type would display intense distress upon separation and cling to the mother but show ambivalent behavior towards her
Problems with using tools like the Strange Situation across cultures:
Each culture views attachment behaviors differently
For example, willingness to explore is seen as secure attachment in America, but in traditional Japanese culture, dependence is valued over independence