Between Subjects Design

Cards (84)

  • Between-subjects design

    A study in which individuals are assigned to only one treatment or experimental condition and each person provides only one score for data analysis
  • Within-subjects design

    An experimental design in which the effects of treatments are seen through the comparison of scores of the same participant observed under all the treatment conditions
  • The American Psychological Association (APA) recommends that the word participants be used when referring to human participants in research, and the word subjects when referring to animals
  • Selecting and recruiting subjects
    • Accurately generalizing results depends on the representativeness of the sample
    • Random sampling procedure is highly desirable to increase external validity
    • Practical limits in recruiting participants (e.g. using university subject pools, asking friends/relatives)
  • Representativeness
    The extent to which a sample mirrors a researcher's target population and reflects its characteristics
  • There is no simple answer to how many subjects are enough for an experiment
  • Determining number of subjects
    • More subjects are needed when individuals in the population are likely to be quite different
    • Larger samples are more likely to mirror the actual state of the population
    • The number of subjects needed depends on the size of the effect produced by the independent variable (effect size)
  • Effect size
    A statistical estimate of the size or magnitude of the treatment effect
  • Two-group design

    The simplest experiment with only one independent variable and two treatment conditions
  • Two-independent-groups design

    Subjects are placed in each of two treatment conditions through random assignment
  • Random assignment
    Every subject has an equal chance of being placed in any of the treatment conditions
  • If a researcher cannot use true random selection in deciding who the subjects of an experiment will be, he or she always uses random assignment to the treatment conditions
  • Even when it is not possible to select your subjects entirely at random, the two-independent groups design can still be used as long as subjects are randomly assigned to each group
  • In an independent groups design, the groups are "independent" of each other. The makeup of one group has no effect on that of the other
  • If subjects are not randomly assigned to treatment groups, confounding can occur
  • Random assignment controls for the differences that exist between subjects before the experiment
  • Random assignment guards against the possibility that subjects' characteristics will vary systematically along with the independent variable
  • Random assignment
    1. Flipping a coin (for two treatment conditions)
    2. Using a random number table (for more than two conditions)
  • Random selection and random assignment are two separate procedures
  • Random assignment is critical to internal validity
  • If the "warm" descriptions were given out first, it could have confounded the results
  • Experimental condition
    Apply a particular value of the independent variable to the subjects and measure the dependent variable
  • Control condition
    Carry out the same procedures as the experimental condition, except for the experimental manipulation
  • No-treatment control condition
    Measure subjects' responses without trying to alter them in any way
  • A no-treatment control condition tells us how subjects ordinarily perform on the dependent measure
  • Researchers must be careful that the control group is not inadvertently engaging in behaviors that would affect the results of the experiment
  • A two-experimental-groups design can be used to look at behavioral differences that occur when subjects are exposed to two different values or levels of the independent variable
  • Forming two independent groups
    1. Randomly assign subjects to experimental and control groups (e.g. by flipping a coin)
    2. Aim to create groups that are equivalent on important subject variables
  • Even if individual subjects differ, the average of the groups should be about equal if random assignment was successful
  • Hypothetical weights of subjects assigned to treatment conditions
    • Experimental Group
    • Control Group
  • Even though individual roaches in each group weigh different amounts, the average weight of the groups is about equal
  • The difference between the groups is not significant; it is not enough to merit concern
  • Assigning subjects at random, we created two groups that are equivalent on an important subject variable
  • Randomization controls for differences we have not identified but that might somehow bias the study
  • The more subjects we have available to assign to treatment conditions, the better the chances are that randomization will lead to equivalent groups of subjects
  • The more subjects we have, the better our chances are of achieving groups that are equivalent on a number of different characteristics
  • Hypothetical weights of new groups
    • Experimental Group
    • Control Group
  • Randomization did not produce comparable groups, the control group turned out to be much heavier than the experimental group
  • The difference in weight is a confounding variable that could contaminate the results
  • Two-matched-groups design

    There are two groups of subjects, but the researcher assigns them to groups by matching or equating them on a characteristic that will probably affect the dependent variable