Moray

Cards (24)

  • Background
    • Attention is a limited resource. When our attention is focused on a certain thing a 'barrier' is put up that stops us from focusing on other things
    • Cherry was interested in how people put up an inatentional barrier at a party when lots of conversations were happening at once. Discovered that this barrier could be broken by your name being said and he called this the cocktail party effect.
  • Dichotic listening
    When headphones are worn by a participant and a different message is played in each ear
  • Shadowing
    Affective instruction:
    • When a person is asked to do something, followed by their name
    Non-affective instruction:
    • When a person is asked to do something not followed by their name
  • Aims
    To further investigate Cherry's theory on inatentional barriers more thoroughly
  • Apparatus
    • Brenell Mark IV stereophonic tape recorder
    • Headphones
  • Experiment 1 - the inatentional barrier sample
    • Undergraduate students
    • Both genders
    • Oxford uni
  • Experiment 1 procedure
    • Participants had to shadow a piece of prose they could hear in one ear - this was the attended message participants were focusing on
    • In the other ear a list of simple words was repeated 35 times. This list was the rejected message.
    • At the end, participants completed a recognition task - they had to indicate what they recognised from a list of 21 words (7 from rejected message, 7 from the attended message and 7 similar words)
  • Experiment 1 results
    • Participants recognised 4.9 words from the shadowed passage
    • 1.9 were recognised from the rejected passage
    • 2.6 similar words were recognised
  • Experiment 1 conclusions
    • Participants recognised far more words from the shadowed passage
    • Almost none of the words from the rejected message were recognised and able to break the inatentional barrier
  • Experiment 2 - affective instructions aim
    • To find out if an affective cue (their name) would break the inattentional barrier.
  • Experiment 2 sample
    • 12 undergraduates
    • Both genders
    • From oxford uni
  • Experiment 2 IV
    • Affective instruction
    • Non-affective instruction
  • Experiment 2 DV

    Whether participants were more likely to hear cue instruction they're not paying attention to if their name is used
  • Experiment 2 procedure
    1. Passages - participants heard 10 passages of light fiction including both affective and non-affective instructions (repeated measures)
    2. Instructions - participants were either told to change ears or stop. They were told to make as few mistakes as possible.
    3. Oder - the instructions were at the start and/or end of the passage
    4. Controls - passages were read at a steady monotone with a pace of 130 per minute and a male voice
  • Experiment 2 results
    Affective instruction
    • Participants heard/followed instruction preceded by their name 20/39 times
    Non-affective instruction
    • Participants heard/followed instruction not preceded by their names 4/36 times
  • Experiment 2 conclusion
    Affective messages (such as names) are able to break the 'inattentional barrier', backing up Cherry's theory
  • Experiment 3 - Pre-warning aim

    To see if a pre-warning would help neutral material break the inattentional barrier.
  • Experiment 3 sample
    • 28 undergraduate students
    • Both genders
    • Oxford uni
    • Split into two groups of 14
  • Experiment three IV

    Warning
    • Participants were told they should memories as many digits as possible
    No warning:
    • Participants were told they would be asked questions at the end of the shadowed passage
  • Experiment three DV
    How many digits participants were able to recall from the rejected message
  • Experiment 3 procedure
    • Participants were asked to shadow one message
    • The messages sometimes contained digits towards the end
    • Digits were sometimes only in the shadowed passage, sometimes only in the rejected passage, sometimes in both and sometimes there were no digits (control)
  • Experiment 3 results

    There was no significant difference between the groups in how many digits they were able to recall from the rejected passage.
  • Experiment 3 conclusions

    Warning do not help neutral info break the barrier. The info must be useful.
  • Overall conclusions
    1. Almost none of the verbal content from a rejected message penetrates a block when attending to another message
    2. 'Important' messages like names can penetrate the barrier
    3. A short list of words cannot be remembered even after being repeated several times
    4. It is difficult to make neutral material important enough to break inattentional barrier