Attention is a limited resource. When our attention is focused on a certain thing a 'barrier' is put up that stops us from focusing on other things
Cherry was interested in how people put up an inatentional barrier at a party when lots of conversations were happening at once. Discovered that this barrier could be broken by your name being said and he called this the cocktail party effect.
Dichotic listening
When headphones are worn by a participant and a different message is played in each ear
Shadowing
Affective instruction:
When a person is asked to do something, followed by their name
Non-affective instruction:
When a person is asked to do something not followed by their name
Aims
To further investigate Cherry's theory on inatentional barriers more thoroughly
Apparatus
Brenell Mark IV stereophonic tape recorder
Headphones
Experiment 1 - the inatentional barrier sample
Undergraduate students
Both genders
Oxford uni
Experiment 1 procedure
Participants had to shadow a piece of prose they could hear in one ear - this was the attended message participants were focusing on
In the other ear a list of simple words was repeated 35 times. This list was the rejected message.
At the end, participants completed a recognition task - they had to indicate what they recognised from a list of 21 words (7 from rejected message, 7 from the attended message and 7 similar words)
Experiment 1 results
Participants recognised 4.9 words from the shadowed passage
1.9 were recognised from the rejected passage
2.6 similar words were recognised
Experiment 1 conclusions
Participants recognised far more words from the shadowed passage
Almost none of the words from the rejected message were recognised and able to break the inatentional barrier
Experiment 2 - affective instructions aim
To find out if an affective cue (their name) would break the inattentional barrier.
Experiment 2 sample
12 undergraduates
Both genders
From oxford uni
Experiment 2 IV
Affective instruction
Non-affective instruction
Experiment 2 DV
Whether participants were more likely to hear cue instruction they're not paying attention to if their name is used
Experiment 2 procedure
Passages - participants heard 10 passages of light fiction including both affective and non-affective instructions (repeated measures)
Instructions - participants were either told to change ears or stop. They were told to make as few mistakes as possible.
Oder - the instructions were at the start and/or end of the passage
Controls - passages were read at a steady monotone with a pace of 130 per minute and a male voice
Experiment 2 results
Affective instruction
Participants heard/followed instruction preceded by their name 20/39 times
Non-affective instruction
Participants heard/followed instruction not preceded by their names 4/36 times
Experiment 2 conclusion
Affective messages (such as names) are able to break the 'inattentional barrier', backing up Cherry's theory
Experiment 3 - Pre-warning aim
To see if a pre-warning would help neutral material break the inattentional barrier.
Experiment 3 sample
28 undergraduate students
Both genders
Oxford uni
Split into two groups of 14
Experiment three IV
Warning
Participants were told they should memories as many digits as possible
No warning:
Participants were told they would be asked questions at the end of the shadowed passage
Experiment three DV
How many digits participants were able to recall from the rejected message
Experiment 3 procedure
Participants were asked to shadow one message
The messages sometimes contained digits towards the end
Digits were sometimes only in the shadowed passage, sometimes only in the rejected passage, sometimes in both and sometimes there were no digits (control)
Experiment 3 results
There was no significant difference between the groups in how many digits they were able to recall from the rejected passage.
Experiment 3 conclusions
Warning do not help neutral info break the barrier. The info must be useful.
Overall conclusions
Almost none of the verbal content from a rejected message penetrates a block when attending to another message
'Important' messages like names can penetrate the barrier
A short list of words cannot be remembered even after being repeated several times
It is difficult to make neutral material important enough to break inattentional barrier