Lasting emotional tie between people such that the individual strives to maintain closeness to the object of attachment and acts to ensure the relationship continues
Attachment
Individuals experience pleasure and security in the presence of others but anxiety and distress when they are gone
Emphasis not only on the physical presence but the "psychological availability"
Ethology
Study of animal behaviour focusing on importance of innate capacities
Ethology Theory
Behaviour involves the formation of an attachment between infant and mother
Adaptive behaviour as promotes survival as leads to proximity between infant and mother
Imprinting
Forming attachment to the first person they see (e.g. ducks can be non-biological entities)
Learning Theory
Attachments are formed through classical associations when parents feed their children
Harlow's Experiment
Baby monkeys fed from the wire mother but cuddled up to the soft cloth mothers and ran to her when scared
Attachment was not due to satisfaction of 'primary' needs (feeding)
Contact Comfort satisfied a primary need
Bowlby's Attachment Theory
Humans have an innate ability to bond with another early on in life
Attachment Formation
1. Phase 1: Pre-attachment (Birth - 2 months)
2. Phase 2: Attachment-in-the-making (2-7 months)
3. Phase 3: Specific Attachments (7 months - 2 years)
4. Phase 4: Multiple attachments (8 months)
5. Phase 5: Formation of a goal-corrected partnership (age 2+)
Strange Situation
Standardized lab experiment paradigm (18-20 months) to measure the organisation of attachment behaviours
Attachment Types
Insecure-Avoidant
Secure
Insecure-Resistant/Ambivalent
Disorganised
Strange Situation Results
Securely attached infants are more sociable with peers, better problem solvers, more persistent and enthusiastic, more socially competent, fewer behavioural problems, and more likely to remember positive emotional events
Factors that shape attachment
Parental behaviour
Child characteristics
Family influences
Cultural differences
Maternal sensitivity
Ability to perceive and interpret children's attachment signals and to respond to them quickly and appropriately
Maternal sensitivity is not a stable trait and changes from one age to another
Temperament hypothesis
Child's temperament plays a role in the attachment relationship
The link between resistant attachment and neonatal irritability is unclear
Maternal deprivation
Separation/loss of mother also failure to develop attachment
Privation
Never been able to form any attachments
Deprivation
Loss or damage to an attachment
Short-term effects of deprivation
1. Protest (Crying)
2. Despair (apathetic, no longer looking for caregiver, self-comforting)
3. Detachment (if situation continues weeks or months, child unresponsive, may ignore caregiver on return)
Bowlby found 86% of delinquent children had, before age of 2, been in foster homes or hospitals, often not visited by families
Rutter found 2,000 boys aged 9-12 were four times more likely to become delinquent if separation related to family discord rather than through illness or death of their mother
Gross early privation (psychological rather than nutritional) resulted in cognitive deficits at age 4 if it went on longer than the first 6 months of the child's life
The evidence is clear: while there is a range of outcomes, early social experience by itself does not predestine the future
The second stage is the attachment process, which begins around six months old when infants start showing signs of separation anxiety.
The first stage is the pre-attachment period, where infants are not yet able to form attachments.
Infants begin to show stranger anxiety between eight and twelve months old, where they prefer familiar people over strangers.
Around nine months old, infants develop an understanding that objects continue to exist even when out of sight, known as object permanence.
By ten months old, infants can distinguish familiar from unfamiliar people based on facial features.
Neonatal
The period immediately after birth, typically the first 28 days of an infant's life.
Link between resistant attachment and neonatal irritability
The relationship between an infant's irritability during the neonatal period and their ability to form secure attachments later in life.