Everyday memory

Cards (38)

  • Factors affecting memory
    • Attention
    • How we encode information (levels of processing)
    • What other people tell us
    • Other information (our prior knowledge and beliefs)
  • Autobiographical memory
    Memories of information related to yourself
  • Cue-word method
    Given a list of words and write down a memory associated with those words
  • Bahrick, Bahrick, & Wittlinger (1975)

    Used highschool yearbooks to test people's memory
  • Bahrick, Bahrick, & Wittlinger (1975) findings

    • 392 graduates
    • Showed yearbook photos, asked for names to pictures
    • 90% accuracy 3 months after graduation
    • 90% 25 years after graduation
    • 80% 45 years after graduation
  • Linton(1975)
    Kept a diary, recorded at least 2 events every day, for 6 years
  • Linton found very little forgetting, and recalled pleasant events better than unpleasant ones
  • Childhood/infantile amnesia
    We typically have no memories from the first 3 years of life
  • Reasons for childhood/infantile amnesia: Development of language, sense of self, etc. Information is processed differently once these have developed, making it hard for us to retrieve things that were processed in a different way. Biology: brain continues to develop during first few years of life (e.g, hippocampus)
  • Reminiscence bump
    Older adults show an increased tendency to recall events from 15-25 years of age
  • Life-script events tend to occur in the 15-25 year age range, which is the typical autobiographical-memory event period
  • Flashbulb memory
    A special type of memory and memory process for surprising and consequential events
  • Examples of flashbulb memories
    • Challenger disaster (1986)
    • 9-11 (2001)
    • Princess diana's death
  • Flashbulb memories are supposedly permanently remembered and have lots of detail, very vivid
  • Brown and Kulik (1977)

    Asked people to describe their memories of significant events, years (13 and 8 years) after the events, and people seemed to have photographic memory
  • We do not know if flashbulb memories are accurate
  • Preferred research protocol for flashbulb memories
    Wait for a surprising consequential event to occur, ask people right then, and then ask them about their memories again months or years later
  • McClosky, Wible, and Cohen (1988)
    Surveyed people right after the disaster and then again 9 months later, found participants' recollections were less specific and somewhat inconsistent after 9 months
  • Neisser and Harsch (1992)
    Surveyed 106 students in PSY 101, just 24 hours after the Challenger event
  • Conway (2008)
    Studied memories from 9-11, found fairly high accuracy and better recall on 9/11/02 than 9/10/02, news media provided cues to help recall
  • Talarico and Rubin (2003)

    Surveyed memory for 9-11 in 54 students one day after the event, retested either 1 week, 6 weeks, or 32 weeks later, found about the same number of details recalled but emotions are higher for flashbulb memories
  • Flashbulb memories are not really any better than, or different from, regular memories (except, perhaps, that we have high confidence in them)
  • Extreme emotion at time of encoding does not necessarily lead to memories that are less prone to forgetting
  • Wagenaar and Groenweg (1990)

    Explored memory of concentration camp survivors 40 years after event, found many details were remembered accurately but survivors also made mistakes
  • The judicial interest is usually in the minute details of eyewitness testimony, such details are not always available, and if they are available, they are not always correct
  • Loftus and Palmer (1974)
    Studied influence of leading questions on memory accuracy, showed participants film of a traffic accident and later tested their memory
  • Loftus, Miller, & Burns (1978)

    Showed participants 30 slides of a red car turning a corner at a stop sign hitting a pedestrian, one question contained misleading information for half of the participants, participants who were given inconsistent information chose the stop sign slide only 41% of the time
  • Post-event information can change the memories of the original event, and different testing methods do not help the original memory resurface
  • Misattribution: The case of Donald Thompson
    A woman identified Donald Thompson as her attacker, but she had actually seen him on TV when the attack happened and confused his face with the attacker
  • Source misattribution: Having a memory, but losing track of where it came from, can lead to confusing imagined events with real events, or self with friends
  • Police questioning and lineup/photo arrays can lead to false memories and overconfidence in witness identifications
  • In psychotherapy, therapists can easily suggest that trauma might have occurred and then the client incorporates that information into a false memory of trauma
  • Combating false memories
    Make a statement of the event as soon as possible, use a cognitive interview (mentally reconstruct the context, report everything, recount in backwards, recount from different perspectives)
  • Mnemonics
    Strategies for remembering, including first letter methods, pegword method, link/story method, method of loci, and keyword mnemonics
  • Mnemonics work well because they make information more meaningful, connect new information to existing knowledge, provide organization, and involve elaboration and imagery
  • False memory lab
    Presents lists of words closely associated with a common 'lure' word, and tests whether participants falsely recall the lure word as if it was studied
  • Participants falsely recall the lure words at a rate similar to the studied words, due to our knowledge and schemas interacting with what we are studying, and spreading activation in semantic memory
  • Memory of the past can incorporate our current knowledge and beliefs