Observational techniques

Cards (16)

  • What are observations?:
    • The experimental method is not always the most suitable way to research certain types of behaviour.
    • Sometimes it is more appropriate to use non experimental methods such as observations.
    • Observational techniques allow us to see what people do without having to ask them. They involve the researcher watching and listening.
  • Types of observation include:
    • Naturalistic and controlled observation
    • Overt and covert observation
    • Participant and non-participant
  • Sometimes observations are used in experiments as a way of measuring the DV. They are therefore not deemed a ‘research method’ but instead a technique used alongside other research methods.
  • Naturalistic:
    • Behaviour studied in natural situation – researcher does not interfere in any way.
    • e.g. observing children playing in the school playground
    Controlled:
    • Occurs when the researchers control some variables
    • Can occur in laboratory or natural situations
    • e.g. watching parent/child interaction through a one way mirror
  • Overt:
    • Participants are aware they are being observed
    • Overt observation can take place in naturalistic and controlled observation
    Covert:
    • Participants are unaware that they are being observed.
    • They may be informed afterwards but have no knowledge until the observation is over.
  • Participant:
    • In a participant observation the observer is also a participant in the activity being studied either with or without the group’s knowledge.
    • e.g. joining a cult to observe the behaviour from the inside
    Non-participant:
    • The observer observes from a distance and does not interact with the p’s
  • Observation strengths:
    • Capture what people actually do – with a low risk of demand characteristics
    • Can therefore give special insight into behaviour
  • Observation limitations:
    • Observer bias – interpreters observation of a situation may be influenced by their expectations
    • Cannot demonstrate causal relationships – although observational techniques can be used in experiments and aid in detecting cause and effect.
    • Provides information about what people do but not about what they think or feel
  • Reliability of observations:
    • Observations should be consistent
    • If two people observed the same activity we need to see if they agree on the behaviours seen – we have to assess inter-observer reliability – above 0.80 is considered good.
    • To calculate the inter-observer reliability: Total agreements ÷ total observations
  • In order to produce high inter-observer reliability, we can do the following things:
    • Clearly operationalise behavioural categories
    • Training observers in observational techniques
    • Pilot the study to review the behaviour checklist and behavioural categories
    • This helps to reduce observer bias (interpreting behaviours from one's own perspective) and in turn, will increase the validity of the results.
  • Observations - External Validity:
    • Strength - High ecological validity- involve more natural behaviours.
    • Limit - Population validity may be a problem, if for example, children are only observed in middle class homes, because we cannot generalise such findings to children from all classes.
  • Observations Internal validity
    Limits:
    • Observations will not be valid if the behaviour checklist is flawed. For example, some observations may belong in more than one category, or some behaviour may not be codeable.
    • The validity of observations is also affected by observer bias – what someone observes is influenced by their expectations. This reduces the objectivity of observations. (This can be solved by using a double blind procedure where the person completing the research is unaware of the hypothesis).
  • Observations reliability
    Limits:
    • Observations can be inconsistent and unreliable if steps aren’t taken to reduce observer bias
  • Inter-Observer Reliability:
    • Observations should be consistent, which means ideally two observers should produce the same record. (This is also known as inter-observer reliability).
    • A general rule is that if there is more than 80% agreement on the observations, the data have inter-observer reliability. (Total Agreements ÷ Total Observations).
    Improving Reliability:
    • Observers should be trained in the use of a coding system/behaviour checklist.
    • They should practice using it and discuss their observations. The investigator can then check the reliability of their observations.
  • Ethical Issues: issues relating to informed consent. Invasion of privacy, in which case participant confidentiality should be respected. The use of one-way mirrors often involves deception.
  • Dealing with Ethical Issues:
    • Ethics committees can be used to approve observational designs, and researchers should consult ethical guidelines.
    • One particular BPS guideline concerns observational research – this type of research is acceptable where those observed would be expected to be observed by strangers.
    • In addition researchers should be aware that it is not acceptable to intrude upon the privacy of individuals who, even in a normally public space, may believe they are not observed.