L4 Neuropsychology

Cards (58)

  • Semantic Memory
    Memory for meaning, facts, and general knowledge about the world
  • Classifications of Memory (Tulving, 1989)
    • Episodic (events)
    • Semantic (meaning)
    • Procedural (motor)
  • Semantic dementia
    • Progressive loss of verbal and non-verbal semantic memory
    • Preservation of other cognitive domains (e.g., working memory, visuo-spatial ability, non-verbal problem solving ability, phonology & syntax)
    • Good orientation and recall of recent events (unlike the amnesia patients)
    • Atrophy to the infero-lateral temporal neocortex (anterior temporal lobes – ATL) with relative preservation of the hippocampus early in the disease
  • Conversation in Semantic Dementia
    • "Oh, I like to do things outside and things in the house"
    • "what's a hobby?"
  • Hierarchical Network Model
    Semantic memory is organized into a series of hierarchical networks, with major concepts represented as nodes and properties/features associated with each concept
  • Warrington (1975) describes cases of progressive semantic degeneration (Semantic Dementia) which follows similar principles to this hierarchical model (lower properties lost first)
  • Hierarchical distance is often confounded with familiarity, and controlling for familiarity greatly reduces the hierarchical distance effect
  • Typicality effect
    Verification is faster for more representative member categories, independent of hierarchical distance
  • Typical items have more commonalities with their category than atypical items, and categories are fuzzier than the Hierarchical Network Model believed
  • Typicality Ratings
    • A German Shepherd is a very doggy dog, while a Pekinese is a less doggy dog
  • SD patients find it hard to match items on conceptual rather than superficial similarities, and can make undergeneralisation and overgeneralisation errors
  • Spreading Activation Model
    Semantic memory is organized by semantic relatedness/distance, with activation spreading from one concept to related concepts, decreasing as it gets further away
  • Semantic priming occurs when a semantically-related word facilitates the processing/identification of a target word
  • Imageability
    The ease with which a word or concept can be visualized
  • Imageability is an important variable implicating semantics, with high imageability words (e.g. CHAIR) processed differently than low imageability words (e.g. JUSTICE)
  • Some semantic dementia patients display a "reverse concreteness effect", performing better on abstract words than concrete words
  • The reverse concreteness effect in semantic dementia is not consistently found across tasks and may be rare
  • Semantic memory impairment
    Can occur due to damage to semantic/conceptual representations themselves or damage to access to those representations
  • Semantic aphasia refers to disorders of semantic memory access, characterized by inconsistency, sensitivity to presentation manipulations, and effects of priming and mis-cueing
  • Category-specific impairments in semantic dementia are very rare, with only a few cases reported of better performance with living or non-living things
  • Proposed explanations for category-specific deficits, such as the sensory-functional theory, have limitations and may be too simplistic
  • Cree and McRae's multiple feature approach can explain various patterns of category-specific deficits by considering the relative reliance on different types of semantic properties
  • Sensory and functional properties
    May be too simplistic a distinction
  • Cree and McRae's (2003) Multiple Feature Approach
    • Argued for subdivisions of the two properties
    • Farah and McClelland's sensory property is divided into: Visual, auditory, taste, and tactile sensations
    • Farah and McClelland's functional property is divided into: Entity behaviors (what a thing does), Functional information (what humans use it for)
  • Deficit patterns that Cree and McRae's (2003) approach can explain
    • Multiple categories consisting of living creatures (impaired by damage to visual motion, visual parts, color)
    • Multiple categories of nonliving things (impaired by damage to function, visual parts)
    • Fruits and vegetables (impaired by damage to color, function, taste, smell)
    • Fruits and vegetables with living creatures (impaired by damage to color)
    • Fruits and vegetables with nonliving things (impaired by damage to function)
    • Inanimate foods with living things (impaired by damage to function, taste, smell)
    • Musical instruments with living things (impaired by damage to sound, color)
  • Verbal listing used to capture features of object categories (e.g. Cree & McRae, 2003) may not always easily capture sensory knowledge
  • Hoffman & Lambon-Ralph (2013) obtained ratings for objects in each sensory-motor modality and found verbal listing overestimated visual form/functional knowledge and underestimated other sensory channels (particularly motion & colour)
  • Hoffman & Lambon-Ralph (2013) found their ratings system generated a novel pattern - a clear distinction between different types of artifact, with mechanical devices (e.g. vehicles, appliances, instruments) distinct from other non-living objects
  • These results suggest man-made artifacts are not a single category
  • Current imaging evidence suggests a complex pattern of neuroanatomical specialisation in object representation, which cannot be accounted for by a simple sensory-functional dichotomy
  • Processing an object's visual characteristics activates ventral occipitotemporal cortex, auditory properties activate posterior superior temporal gyrus, somatosensory properties activate post-central gyrus, gustatory information activates medial orbitofrontal cortex, visual motion processing associates with middle and superior temporal gyri, and visual form and colour rely on posterior fusiform
  • Praxic processing is associated with the parietal lobule, but appears to be neurally distinct from information about the object's function
  • Some patients present with a focal impairment of a particular semantic category, but other researchers have argued that semantic representations may be differentiated into different 'kinds' of information or different 'modalities' of information, rather than explicit categories
  • Not all semantic impaired cases have category specific impairments - semantic dementia cases have a more general semantic impairment
  • Semantic dementia cases have focal atrophy to the anterior temporal lobes (ATL), leading to the suggestion this region is a 'semantic hub'
  • Early fMRI studies did not show ATL activation, likely due to signal drop-out, but later studies using distortion-corrected fMRI have confirmed ATL activation in semantic tasks
  • rTMS studies inhibiting left or right ATL increased picture naming RTs and affected semantic judgement tasks, with all types of concepts (living/nonliving) affected, suggesting a domain general system
  • tDCS studies have also shown consequences of ATL stimulation/inhibition on semantic processing
  • Pobric et al. (2010) found rTMS to ATL disrupted living & non-living, manipulable/non-manipulable items, consistent with a 'hub' principle, while rTMS to inferior temporal lobes induced a nonliving worse than living pattern and greater impairment for highly manipulable items
  • Hurley et al. (2012) found semantic dementia cases showed 'blunting' of intra-category boundaries linked to left ATL atrophy, suggesting items are easier to understand "at the generic rather than specific level of representation"