broadbent (1958) suggested that the world is composed of many more sensations that can be handled by the perceptual and cognitive capabilities of the human observer -> humans selectively attend to only some information and 'tune out' the rest. -> attention is the result of a limited capacityinformation processing system
cherry (1953) first used the shadowing technique as a form of dichotic listening - people presented with two or more simultaneous 'messages' and are instructed to process and respond only to one of them
cherry (1953)
ppts asked to repeat 1 message out of 2 that they were listening to - messages presented at the same time and done in two ways
binaural task - both messages played in both ears at the same time
dichotic task - one message played in one ear and the other message played in the other year
findings - binaural task was significantly harder, dichotic task ppts could separate the messages effectively but couldn't recall the contents of the ignore/unattended message
the cocktail party phenomenon - ability to focus one's attention to a specific stimulus whilst filtering out a range of other stimuli e.g. conversations in a cocktail party -> can focus attention on a particular conversation
divided attention - dual-task technique where people are asked to attend and respond to both (or all) the messages -> deliberately divides people's attention
experiment 1 aim
test cherry's findings in relation to the amount of information recognised in the rejected message
experiment 1 design
laboratory experiment
repeated measures design
experiment 1 sample
number of ppts not recorded
both males and females
all ppts were undergraduate or research workers
experiment 1 procedure
dichotic listening task
attended message - a prose message that was shadowed
rejected message - a short list of simple words repeated 35 times, played at the same time as the prose message
IV - dichotic listening test, recognition test
DV - ppts asked to recall all they could from the unattended message, 30 seconds later given a recognition test using similar material as a control
3 conditions - shadowed message, rejected message and a control
experiment 1 results
little evidence of words from the rejected message being recognised
mean recognition rate was lower for the shadowed message and even lower for words not present in either list
previous appearance of words
shadowed message - 4.9
rejected message - 1.9
new words (neither messages) - 2.6
experiment 2 aim
test Cherry's findings in relation to the effect of hearing your name in the unattended message
experiment 2 design
laboratory experiment
repeated measures design
experiment 2 sample
12 participants
both males and females
all ppts were undergraduate or research workers
experiment 2 procedure
dichotic listening task
attended message - 10 short passages of light fiction, each being different
rejected message - set of instructions given to the ppts, some were affective (emotional) and some were non-affective (neutral). affective instructions had the ppt's name - looked at efficiency of the attentional block
IV - whether the instructions were prefixed by the ppts name or not
DV - number of affective instructions that ppts heard
experiment 2 procedure 2
ppts told that their responses would be recorded and objective was to shadow each of the 10 passages - were told that they were scored and to try to make as few mistakes as possible
10 passages had 2 instructions given at the same time - first was at the start of the passage, second was during the passage
experiment 2 results
significant differences between affective and non-affective instructions - the names caused the instructions to be heard which broke through the attentional barrier
most ppts ignored instructions presented in the passages they were shadowing as they thought it was an attempt to distract them
affective condition - 20 out of 39 messages were heard
non-affective condition - 4 out of 36 messages were heard (3 results were rejected as ppts started paying attention to the rejected message)
experiment 3 aim
test Cherry's findings in relation to the effect of instructions to identify a specific target in the rejected message
experiment 3 design
laboratory experiment
independent measures design
experiment 3 sample
2 x 14 ppts
both males and females
all ppts were undergraduate or research workers
experiment 3 procedure
dichotic listening task
attended message - experimental message that was shadowed
rejected message - either an experimental message (had digits within it) or a control message (similar message without digits)
ppts heard
2 experimental messages OR
1 experimental message and 1 control message
experiment 3 procedure 2
IV1 - whether digits were inserted into both messages or only one
IV2 - whether ppts had to answer questions about the shadowed message or whether ppts had to merely remember all the numbers they could
DV - number of digits correctly reported
all ppts shadowed an experimental message
one group told beforehand that they would be asked questions about the content of the shadowed message
other group told they should remember all the numbers they could
experiment 3 results
mean number of digits reported when ppts were told they would be asked about the content was not significantly different
ppts could not be primed to listen for digits in the rejected message unlike the use of their own name
stimulus not important enough
experiment 3 overall conclusions
In a situation where a participant directs his attention to the reception of a message from one ear, and rejects a message from the other ear, almost none of the verbal content of the rejected message is able to penetrate the block set up.
A short list of simple words presented as the rejected message shows no trace of being remembered.
‘important’ messages, such as a person’s own name, can penetrate the block
it is very difficult to make ‘neutral’ material important enough to break through the block set up in dichotic shadowing.
research methods - strength
laboratory experiment, possible to control extraneous variables leading to higher validity
ppts had the loudness in each ear individually matched to ensure differences were not caused by volume
messages were recorded to ensure they were spoken at a constant speed without expression
how loudly ppt's name was spoken was checked that it had not been stressed
sample - strength
students and research workers may be no different in relation to selective attention skills from the general population
sample included both males and females - possible that there is a gender difference in selective attention
sample - weakness
sampling method may be non-representative as ppts were students and research workers -> cannot be generalised
quantitative data - strengths
allows easy comparison between conditions - digits cannot be noticed even when expected but one's name can be detected some of the time in the rejected message when not expected
quantitative data - weakness
no analysis of qualitative data - difficult to process detecting particular stimuli in the rejected message is unconscious -> ppts unable to say or know why they succeeded in detecting or failing to do so
ethics - strength
few to no ethical issues - the task, materials and findings were unlikely to cause distress
ppts knew what was going to happen - no deception
reliability - strength
controlling variables such as volume, use of headphones - raised reliability and standardised procedure between ppts and conditions to allow for replication
reliability - weakness
some differences in ppts' experiences - some only heard and responded to the instruction to change ears so passage intended to be shadowed then became the rejected message
one ppt spontaneously reverted to shadowing the original ear without noticing
validity - strength
level of control ensured greater recall of the name than other words was due to the IV not to extraneous variables
ecologically valid to some extent - dichotic listening represents the everyday situation of trying to follow one source of information such as conversation while trying to distract noise, chatter or dialogue
validity - weakness
lack of ecological validity as not realistic - rarely need to continuously listen and repeat a message
Practical applications
Driver safety (Moray, 1990)
Directional attention to one's own name as a test of consciousness in long-term coma patients (Cheng et al, 2013)
Ability to switch attention between messages to understand children with listening but not hearing disorders (Dharmani et al, 2013)
Study was central to many years of research into the process to selectively attend to some inputs and the nature of stimuli that can overcome the attentional block