Motor cortex - location for awareness of what we are doing within our environment (consciousness) and responsible for generation of some voluntary motor movements, such as walking or running
Parietal lobe
Somatosensory cortex - location for sensory information from the skin
Occipital lobe
Visual cortex - location for visual information
Temporal lobe
Auditory cortex - location for auditory ability which analyses speech-based information (language comprehension)
Symptom of damaged frontal lobe
Loss of control of fine movements
Symptom of damaged temporal lobe
Unable to understand a question
Impairment in ability to group meaning of spoken words (Wernike'saphasia)
Symptom of damaged parietal lobe
Difficulty identifying a sensation'slocation and type
Difficulty recognising objects by touch
Lack of awareness of own body, limbs and positioning in space
Symptom of damaged occipital lobe
Cannot see even though the eyes themselves are functioning normally - cortical blindness (some people with cortical blindness are unaware they cannot see)
Wernike's area
In the left temporal lobe - language comprehension
Broca's area
In the left frontal lobe - speech production
Cerebellum
Receives information from sensory systems, spinal cord and other parts of the brain - regulates motor movements
Coordinates voluntary motor movements, such as posture, balance, coordination and speech
Localisation of function
Theory that there are specific areas of the cerebral cortex associated with particular cognitive functions.
Argued for by Broca and Wernike due to their research in brain damage and post-mortems
Phineas Gage case study
Prior to this (19th century), the brain was considered to work in a holistic way
Phineas Gage (1823-1860)
Iron tamping pole went through his head when he was fixing a railroad
Led to a personality change where he became more irritable and rude
Dr Bigelow thought there was no localisation and believed that the rest of Phineas' brain had compensated for the parts destroyed in the accident
Dr Harlow believed that there was localisation in the brain and that areas involved in planning, reasoning and control had been damaged
Peterson (1958)
Used brain scans to demonstrate how Wernike's area was active during a listening task and Broca's area was active during a reading task
Suggests that listening is localised in Wernike's area and reading is localised in Broca's area - supported by evidence from fMRI and ERP scans
Plasticity application
Could be applied to neurorehabilitation where physical therapy is used with brain-damaged patients to help recover lost functions
Lashley's law of equipotentiality suggests that surviving brain circuits work together to achieve the same neurological action
Completeness - higher cognitive functions are not localised
Lashley (1950) suggests that the basic motor and sensory functions were localised but higher mental functions were not
Claimed that intact areas of the cortex could take over responsibility for specific cognitive functions following injury to the area normally responsible for that function
This view suggests that the effects of damage to the brain would be determined by the extent rather than the localisation of damage
Phineas Gage case study for localisation of the brain
Iron tamping pole went through his head
Before the accident, he had been kind and reserved but after, his personality changed to being boisterous, rude and grossly blasphemous
Damasio et al found that his frontal lobe had been damaged
This suggests that personality and emotions may have been localised to the frontal lobe
However, does not account for environmental factors such as loss of job or feelings about accident and excruciating pain rather than actual localisation of the brain - unique - low generalisability
Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)
Works by detecting changes in both blood oxygenation and flow that occurs as a result of neural activity in specific parts of the brain
When a brain area is more active, it consumes more oxygen and, to meet this increased demand, blood flow is directed to the active area (haemodynamic response)
Produces 3D images showing which parts of the brain are involved in a particular mental process and this had important implications for our understanding of localisation of function
Strengths of fMRI
Does not rely on use of radiation, unlike other scanning techniques, such as PET
Is virtually risk-free, non-invasive and straightforward to use - if administered correctly
Produces images that have very high spatial resolution, depicting detail by the millimetre, and providing a clear picture of how the brain is localised - means it can safely provide a clear picture of brain activity
Limitations of fMRI
Expensive compared to other neuroimaging techniques
Poor temporal resolution as there is around a 5 second time-lag behind the image on screen and the initial firing of neuronal activity - may not represent moment to moment brain activity
Electroencephalogram (EEG)
Measures electrical activity within the brain via electrodes that are fixed to an individual's scalp using a skull cap
The scan recording represents the brainwave patterns that are generated from the action of thousands of neurons, providing an overall account of brain activity
Often used by clinicians as a diagnostic toll as unusual arrhythmic patterns of activity may indicate neurological abnormalities such as epilepsy, tumours or some sleep disorders
Strengths of EEGs
Has been useful in studying the stages of sleep and in the diagnosis of conditions such as epilepsy
Has extremely high temporal resolution - can accurately detect brain activity at a resolution of a single millisecond - shows real-world usefulness of this technique
Limitations of EEGs
The generalised nature of the information received
EEG signal is not useful pinpointing the exact source of neural activity - does not allow researchers to distinguish between activities originating in different but adjacent locations
Event-related potentials (ERPs)
Isolated neural responses associated with specific sensory, cognitive and motor events that are averaged statistically - all extraneous brain activity from original EEG recording is filtered out leaving only those responses that relate to the specific thing being studied
Specifically types of brainwave that are triggered by particular events
Research has revealed many different forms of ERP and how those are linked to cognitive processes such as attention and perception
Strengths of ERPs
Partly addresses limitations of EEGs
Bring much more specificity to the measurement of neural processes that could ever be achieved using raw EEG data
Excellent temporal resolution, especially when compared to neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI - means ERPs are frequently used to measure cognitive functions and deficits such as the allocation of attentional resources and the maintenance of working memory
Limitations of ERPs
Lack of standardisation in ERP methodology between different research studies which makes it difficult to confirm findings
In order to establish pure data in ERP studies, background 'noise' and extraneous material must be completely eliminated - not always easy to achieve
Post-mortem examinations
Involves analysis of a person's brain following their death
Likely conducted on individuals who had a rare disorder and experienced unusual deficits in cognitive processes or behaviour during their lifetime
Areas of damage examined after death as a means of establishing the likely cause of the affliction the person experienced - may involve comparison with a neurotypical brain in order to ascertain the extent of difference
Strengths of post-mortems
Was vital in providing a foundation for early understanding of key processes in the brain
Broca and Wernike both relied on post-mortems in establishing links between language and behaviour decades before neuroimaging became a possibility
Also used to study HM's brain to identify areas of damage, which could be associated with his memory deficits - they continue to provide useful information
Hemispheric lateralisation
The fact that the same mental processes in the brain are mainly specialised to either the left or right hemisphere
Split-brain research
Research that studies individuals who have been subjected to the surgical separation of the 2 hemispheres of the brain as a result of severing the corpus callosum
Corpus callosum
A bundle of nerve fibres which joins two halves of the brain
Sperry (1968)
Quasi-experiment involving a unique group of 11 individuals who had all undergone a commissurotomy (cutting the corpus callosum and other tissues connecting the 2 hemispheres down the middle to separate the hemispheres and control frequent and severe epileptic seizures)
Meant that for these patients, the main communication line between the 2 hemispheres was removed, allowing Sperry to see the extent to which the 2 hemispheres were specialised for certain functions
Sperry conclusions
The left hemisphere was the primary hemisphere for the processing of language - understanding
The right hemisphere was able to recognise words, make mental associations, process emotional reactions and solve simple arithmetic - better than the left hemisphere at spatial skills
Plasticity
The brain's tendency to change and adapt as a result of experience and new learning - generally involves the growth of new connections
Functional recovery
A form of plasticity - following damage through trauma, the brain's ability to redistribute or transfer functions usually performed by damaged areas to other undamaged areas
Plasticity AO1
During infancy, brain experiences rapid growth in the number of synaptic connections it has - twice as many as there are in the adult brain
Cognitive pruning happens where unused connections are deleted and frequently used connections are strengthened in the process
Bridging occurs where new links are made between neurons - shows brain in a continual state of change from growth in early years to change and refinement in adulthood as we learn and experience
However, can be negative (e.g. prolonged drug use leading to poorer cognitive functioning)
Functional recovery AO1
Following physical injury or other forms of trauma, unaffected areas are sometimes able to adapt or compensate for those areas that are damaged - an example of neural plasticity
Can happen quickly after trauma (spontaneous recovery) and then slow down after several weeks or months
The brain is able to rewire and reorganise itself by forming new synaptic connections close to the area of damage
Secondary neural pathways that would not typically be used to carry out certain functions are 'unmasked' to enable functioning to continue - supported by structural changes
Structural changes that support functional recovery
Axon sprouting
Denervation supersensitivity
Recruitment of homologous areas
Axon sprouting
New nerve endings grow and connect with undamaged areas