to see if children developed consevation skills at an earlierage than piaget found , if the change to the materials was accidental.
study design
A laboratory study in which there was some control of possible extraneous variables. All procedures were standardised to ensure the study could be replicated easily. Eighty children between the ages of four and six took part in the study.
Method
The children were shown two rows of counters and asked if there was the sameamount in each row. Then a glove puppet called NaughtyTeddy' made an appearance. Naughty Teddy accidentallymessed up one row of counters in front of the child. The messed-up row was spreadout to look longer than the otherrow. The experimenter pretended to be really cross with NaughtyTeddy and told it off. The child was then asked if there were the same amount of counters in each row
Results
62 per cent of the four-to six-year-oldsstated that there was still the sameamount of counters in each row, therefore they couldconserve. Only 16 per cent of study, when the adult made the change to the counters,four-to six-year-oldsanswered the question correctly in Piaget's conservation of number
Conclusion
Children younger than the age of seven can conserve if the change to the materials is seen to be accidental. When NaughtyTeddymesses up the row of counters and spreads them out, younger children know that the amount of countershasnotchanged.
Other psychologists have also found that children develop conservation skills before seven years of age
• It showed that contrary to Piaget's conclusions, children younger than seven can conserve.
Limitations of the study
• Over 30 per cent of children still failed to conserve when Naughty Teddy' made the change.
The study was replicated by another psychologist who found that althoughmore children could conserve when Naughty Teddy' was used, the results were notashigh as McGarrigle and Donaldson had found.