Collins & Quillian (1969) model of semantic memory
Word meanings are represented in a semantic network, with each word consisting of conceptual 'nodes' that are connected to each other such that activation of a single node passes activation on to other interconnected nodes
Collins & Loftus (1975) revision of the semantic memory model
Included the principle of 'spreading activation' such that connections between nodes in the model vary both in activation strength and relative distance
Warrington (1975) used the hierarchical model as the basis for understanding a patient with a progressive deterioration of semantic memory (Semantic Dementia)
As the patient declines, they don't simply lose all information about a concept at once, instead they lose fine-grained item specific information first, retaining higher order semantic information about items
Patients with disruption of semantic memory are typically far superior at accessing conceptual information specific to words that are highly imageable or concrete relative to low imageability or abstract words
The presence of cases with the reverse concreteness effect remains controversial, but there have been more recent studies in which such a pattern has been observed
McClelland, McNaughton & O'Reilly (1995) Complementary Learning Systems Theory
Semantic information can be obtained by two complementary processes: a slow-learning semantic system and a fast learning system in the medial temporal lobes that rapidly generates connections within this system
The anterior part of the inferior temporal gyrus is considered the terminus of the ventral visual processing stream, the middle temporal gyrus integrates input from multiple modalities, and the superior temporal gyrus is important for speech and auditory perception
Semantic features are stored within particular primary association areas, with 'convergence zones' in the temporal lobe acting as a 'relay' station for conceptual representations
The anterior temporal lobes act as a 'hidden layer' that drives amodal semantic representations, helping determine semantic similarity independent of surface features
Patients can present with impaired comprehension or semantic processing of a particular category of items (e.g., living things) relative to other categories
Sartori & Job (1988) suggested that on the basis of their case's poor performance at visually processing objects, that apparent category specific deficits may not implicate semantic memory, rather they reflect damage to 'structural descriptions' of visual objects (in other words category specific deficits may well arise due to a pre-semantic impairment)
Funnell & Sheridan (1992) and Stewart, Parkin & Hunkin, (1992) pointed out that previous studies reporting a living/non-living dissociation did match testing stimuli for name frequency, but not for an additional variable considered to be vital in psycholinguistic research, namely familiarity
It was shown that apparent category specific impairments demonstrated by their cases disappeared when other cognitive variables such as familiarity, visual complexity and structural similarity were sufficiently matched across living and non-living category stimuli
The notion of personal familiarity is very difficult to quantify, since it is clear dependent on the types of things individuals are exposed to, interested in etc
Familiarity (on the basis of personal experience) may even differ across gender –lines; Albanese, Capitani, Barbarotto & Laiacona, (2000) reviewed cases with greater living or non-living impairments and found that all cases with greater impairment of animals versus fruits were female, whereas all cases of the reverse were male
Farah, Meyer & McMullen (1995) demonstrated in their patient that although category specific (inanimate > animate) differences in performance could be explained by familiarity and visual complexity on the basis of a single testing session, the presence of a category specific impairment (over and above possible artifactual effects) was statistically reliable across several testing sessions
The very first patients reported with category specific impairments had survived herpes simplex encephalitis (Warrington & Shallice, 1984; Sartori & Job, 1988; Silveri & Gainotti, 1988) and in most cases damage implicated bilateral temporal, hippocampal and possibly even frontal regions (Sartori, Job, Miozzo, Zago & Marchiori, 1993)
Tippett, Grossman & Farah (1996) reported no category specific impairments in their sample of 14 probable AD cases using Funnell & Sheridan's well balanced stimuli, suggesting this may reflect the diffuse nature of neurodegeneration in AD