Atkinson and Shiffrin 1968, a theoretical cognitive model of how the memory system processes information
Sensory register
1. Receives raw sense impressions
2. Attention passes info to short-term memory
3. Coding is modality specific
4. Capacity is very large
5. Duration is very short (250 milliseconds)
Short-term memory
1. Receives info from sensory register by paying attention or from long-term memory by retrieval
2. Keeps information by repeating maintenance rehearsal or passing to long-term memory
3. Coding is acoustic
4. Duration is approximately 18 seconds
5. Capacity is 7 plus or minus 2 items
Long-term memory
1. Very long duration, permanent memory storage
2. Theoretically unlimited capacity
3. Coded semantically in the form of meaning
4. Information must be passed back to short-term memory to be used
Words at the start and end of word lists were more easily recalled (Primacy and recency effect)
Recall of a random row of a 12x12 grid flashed for 120th of a second was 75%, suggesting all the rows were stored in sensory register
Immediate recall was worse for acoustically similar words and recall after 20 minutes was worse for semantically similar words, suggesting short-term memory is coded acoustically and long-term memory is coded semantically
Capacity of short-term memory
Average 7 items for letters, 9 for numbers (Jacobs)
Duration of short-term memory
Less than 10% recall of a 3-letter trigram after 18 seconds with an interference task (Peterson and Peterson)
Capacity of long-term memory
75% recall of critical details after 1 year, 45% after 5 years (Wagner's diary study)
Duration of long-term memory
90% recall of school friends' names after 15 years, 80% after 48 years (Bahrick)
Cognitive tests of memory like the MSM are often highly artificial, low in mundane realism, and conducted in lab environments, so findings may not generalize to real-world memory use
Types of long-term memory
Declarative (explicit, conscious)
Non-declarative (implicit, unconscious)
Episodic (experiences and events)
Semantic (facts and knowledge)
Procedural (skills and habits)
Patients with hippocampal damage had episodic amnesia but intact semantic memory, suggesting they use different brain regions (Vargha-Khadem)
Clive Wearing had retrograde amnesia for episodic and semantic memories, but could gain new procedural memories, suggesting they use different brain areas
Generalizing findings from idiographic case studies to explain memory in the wider population is problematic, as other unknown issues could be unique to that individual
Working memory model
Baddeley and Hitch 1974, a theoretical counter model of information processing that replaced the STM store in the MSM
Working memory model components
1. Central executive (controls attention and filters information)
3. Visuospatial sketchpad (processes visual and spatial information)
4. Episodic buffer (general store to hold and combine information)
Performing two visual tasks or a visual and verbal task simultaneously is better when they use different processing, suggesting the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad are separate systems (Baddeley)
Brain injury patient KF had selective impairment to verbal short-term memory but not visual functioning, suggesting the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad are separate processes in the brain (Shallice and Warrington)
The working memory model seems more accurate than the short-term memory component of the MSM in describing how memory is used as an active processor
Memory tasks used in research often lack mundane realism and may not generalize to real-world memory use
The central executive concept in the working memory model needs further development, and the inclusion of the episodic buffer is part of this (Baddeley)
It is impossible to directly observe the processes described in memory models, so inferences and assumptions must be made that could be incorrect
Interference theory
We forget because our long-term memories become confused or disrupted by other information
Types of interference
Proactive interference (old information disrupts new)
Retroactive interference (new information disrupts old)
Similarity interference (more likely when information is similar)
Time sensitivity interference (less likely with longer gaps)
Retrieval failure due to absence of cues
Information is in long-term memory but forgetting happens due to lack of appropriate prompts or context
The encoding specificity principle states that context-dependent and state-dependent cues in the environment and internal state can aid memory retrieval
Retroactive interference
New information disrupts old information
Proactive interference
Previously learned information causes confusion in the coding of later information
Interference only explains forgetting when two sets of information are similar and one learned closer together in time
Context dependent cues
Aspects of our external environment that work as cues to memory
State dependent cues
Aspects of our internal environment that work as cues to memory
Category or organizational dependent cues
Providing cues that relate to the organization or category of memories
The most effective cues have fewer things associated with them, the lack of organization cues inhibits memory
Retroactive interference
Adding new street names to memory makes recalling old street names harder
Proactive interference
Previously learned word combinations cause confusion in the coding of later word lists
Interference may only explain a temporary loss of information, not a permanent loss
Research into forgetting has practical applications for students developing effective revision strategies
Reconstructive memory
Memory is not an accurate recording of events, it's reconstructed in recalling