Loftus and Palmer's PPs watched film clips in a lab, a very different experience from witnessing a real event (e.g. less stressful)
What eyewitnesses remember has important consequences in the real world, but PP's responses in research do not matter in the same way (so research PPs are less motivated to be accurate)
This suggests that the memory itself is distorted through contamination by misleading post-event discussion, rather than the result of memory conformity
Demand characteristics: Lab studies have identified misleading information as a cause of inaccurate EWT, partly by being able to control variables. But Zaragoza and McCloskey argue that many answers given by PPs in lab studies are due to demand characteristics. PPs usually want to be helpful and not let the researcher down. So they guess when they are asked a question they don't know the answer to.
Weapon focus effect
Eyewitness accuracy is significantly poorer when a weapon is present, compared to other objects
The study by Johnson and Scott may not have tested anxiety, as the reason participants focused on the weapon may have been because they were surprised at what they saw rather than scared
Counterpoint: Christianson and Hubinette interviewed their PPs several months after the event (4-15 months). The researchers therefore had no control over what happened to their PPs in the intervening time (e.g. post-event discussions). The effects of anxiety may have been overwhelmed by these other factors and impossible to assess by the time the PPs were interviewed. Therefore it is possible that a lack of control over confounding variables may be responsible for these findings, invalidating their support.
Problems with inverted-U theory: The inverted-U theory appears to be a reasonable explanation of the contradictory findings linking anxiety with both increased and decreased eyewitness recall. On the other hand it ignores the fact that anxiety has many elements - cognitive, behavioural, emotional and physical. It focuses on just the last of these (physical arousal) and assumes this is the only aspect linked to EWT. But the way we think about the stressful situation (i.e. cognitive) may also be important
Support for the effectiveness of the CI: One strength of the CI is evidence that it works. For example, a meta-analysis by Kohnken et al. combined data from 55 studies comparing the CI (and the ECI) with the standard police interview. The CI gave an average 41% increase in accurate information compared with the standard interview. Only 4 studies in the analysis showed no difference between the types of interview. This shows that the CI is an effective technique in helping witnesses to recall information that is stored in memory (available) but not immediately accessible.
Counterpoint: Kohnken et al. also found an increase in the amount of inaccurate information recalled by PPs. This was a particular issue in the ECI, which produced more incorrect details than the CI. Cognitive interviews may sacrifice the quality of EWT (i.e. accuracy) in favour of quantity (amount of details). This means that police officers should treat eyewitness evidence from CIs/ ECIs with caution
Cognitive Interview (CI)
Not all of its elements are equally effective or useful
Using a combination of report everything and reinstate the context produced better recall than any of the other elements or combination of them